Epigenetics In Transgenerational Responses To Environmental Impacts: Facts And Gaps

Author(s):

 Claudine Junien Claudine Junien
INRA, UMR1198 Biologie du Développement et Reproduction, F-78350 Jouy-en-Josas, France claudine.junien@jouy.inra.fr
View Author’s Full Biography

 

Summary

There is increasing interest in non-genetic and non-cultural mechanisms transferring a memory of parental exposure to various environments and determining the reactivity of subsequent generations to their environment during their lifetimes. However, fundamental questions remain about the nature, roles and relative importance of epigenetic marks and processes, non-coding RNAs, or other mechanisms, and their persistence over generations. No model incorporating the various transmission systems, their nature, respective impact and mechanisms, whether direct or indirect, their cross-talk and windows of sensitivity as a function of the sex of the parent and offspring, has yet been built.

 

Revisiting the theories of J.B. Lamarck in the light of epigenetics

Our capacity to respond to the various challenges and hazards of life, and to stress and risks of disease, during childhood and adulthood, depends on the health and human capital with which we are born [1]. These observations underlie the concept of “developmental origins of health and disease” (DOHaD) [2]. The notion that non-genetic and non-cultural mechanisms are able to transmit the memory of exposure to diverse environmental conditions to subsequent generations, conditioning their reactions, has excited considerable interest and has brought the long-criticised proposals of J.B. Lamarck back into the limelight (box).

 

Box

The carving behind the base shows Jean-Baptiste Lamarck and his daughter, Aménaïde Cornélie. It bears the inscription: “La postérité vous admirera, elle vous vengera, mon père” (Posterity will admire you and avenge you, father).

Jean-Baptiste Pierre Antoine de Mont, Chevalier de Lamarck (1774-1829) was a French biologist/zoologist and anatomist who made a major contribution to the classification of life forms through his four laws:

First law: Life, through its own forces, tends to increase continually the volume of any body that it possesses and to extend the dimensions of its parts to a limit that it itself defines.

Second law: The production of a new organ in the body of an animal results from a new need that occurs and continues to be felt and a new movement that needs to be born and maintained.

Third law: The development of the organs and their force of action is constantly consistent with the use of these organs.

Fourth law: All that has been acquired, traced or changed in the organisation of individuals, during their lifetime, is conserved by the generation concerned and is transmitted to the new individuals produced by those that have experienced these changes.

Non-genetic transmission processes are often described as Lamarckian because they raise the possibility of inheriting characters acquired by previous generations. The key characteristics of Lamarckian mechanisms are: 1) an environmental factor directly causing “heritable” changes; 2) the changes induced target a limited set of cell components of functional relevance; 3) the changes provide a specific adaptation to the initial challenge. However, the proof-of-concept for a role of epigenetic processes in Lamarckian evolution remains tenuous or fragmentary. The fourth law, which was formulated two centuries ago, may seem to go against the finding that the epigenetic marks carried by the gametes are extensively erased after fertilization, ensuring a state of totipotency that should not allow the passage of information about the experiences of parents or ancestors. However, Lamarck began from the notion that a change in environment provokes changes in the needs of the organisms living in that environment, in turn triggering changes in their behaviour. These changes in behaviour lead to greater or lesser use of the organ concerned, resulting in changes in the size of the organ (increases in size or disappearance) over time and generations.

epigenetics-1

 

The consequences of environmental factors such as diet, stress, chemical products or other psychoaffective, geographic, political or socioeconomic influences can simultaneously affect at least three generations — the mother and father (F0), their children (F1) and their grandchildren (F2) — through somatic and/or germline changes in the F1 generation (Figure 1) [1].

epigenetics-2

Figure 1. Sex-specific transmission of the memory of exposure to environmental factors to subsequent generations. Environmental factors, including nutrition, psychosocial stress, toxins, endocrine disruptors, tobacco, alcohol, and microbiota, affect individual (F0) epigenetic landscapes and, therefore gene pathways and networks, in ways that differ between the sexes. For example, maternal and paternal exposures before the conception of their offspring can modify gamete quality and information about these exposures can be transmitted to the next (F1) generation. In addition, the consequences of maternal (F0) exposure during pregnancy (stress, metabolism, diet, hormonal changes, etc.) can be transmitted from the maternal to the foetal compartment via the placenta, in a sex-specific manner, with effects on F1 tissue development. The programming of somatic tissues can lead to changes in long-term health outcomes in the first generation. Moreover, primordial germ cells, which develop and undergo reprogramming during foetal development, can also be affected by the F0 maternal environment and may transmit genetic and epigenetic information to the F2 generation. These influences are transmitted differently by the maternal and paternal lineages. In particular, multigenerational exposure in the maternal lineage can be seen in the F0, F1 and F2 generations, with a transgenerational phenotype observed in the F3, whereas, in the paternal lineage, multigenerational exposure concerns the F0 and F1 generations, and a transgenerational phenotype is seen in the F2 and F3 generations. From [55].

 

 

Our experiences in utero and during the first two years of life (the 1000-days concept) are a clear determinant of our health capital. However, the phases preceding conception, beginning with gametogenesis and distinguishing between effects on the primordial germline cells, the gametes, are also important and must be taken into account.

The main challenge is identifying the means by which this information linked to environmental consequences or a physiopathological change is carried and transferred from one generation to another. The principal avenues of research have converged on certain regions of DNA (genes, repeated sequences, etc.) in which epigenetic marks may partially escape from the successive phases of reprogramming — erasure, establishment and maintenance — linked to zygotic erasure after fertilisation. It has been suggested that these regions could carry or mediate persistent changes in chromatin configuration following exposure to an environmental factor. The significance of non-coding RNAs (short and long) is becoming increasingly evident [3].Also, it is clearly emerging that many new vectors, such as exosomes, prions, metabolites, pathogens, chemical substances and the maternal microbiota, also have non-negligible roles [4].

 

Transgenerational responses to programming: a vicious circle or resilience?

The programming occurring, always under the influence of the environment during development, or inherited from parents, may be seen as the “first event.” It often confers no more than a latent state, a sensitivity to a “second event”, and is revealed later by an accumulation of environmental risk factors, leading to a threshold being crossed. It does not, therefore, strictly correspond to long-term “effects”. Instead, it relates to all elements conditioning the “response capacity” (increases or decreases) of programmed tissues or organs conferring a predisposition to vulnerability or resilience. Throughout these processes, it is also dependent on genetic background.

Most phenotypic studies have been limited to explorations of the system disturbed by parental or ancestral exposure: metabolism for nutritional exposure or behaviour for stress exposure, for example. However, depending on the stage of exposure, these disturbances may affect different systems, or even all systems. Thus, paternal exposure to stress has been linked not only to behavioural problems, but also to metabolic problems in descendants [5]. The phenotyping of descendants in animal models generally focuses on deleterious effects, thus ignoring the non-negligible proportion of “resistant” subjects with positive adaptive responses to the exposure of their parents or grandparents [6, 7]. Nevertheless, studies in Caenorhabitis elegans and Drosophila have revealed the existence, in some cases, of an ability to adapt, or resilience. Programming can endow networks of genes with a capacity to respond more rapidly to an environmental challenge [1]. Responses opposite to the initial effects may also be observed. For example, in the Overkalix cohort (Sweden), male malnutrition before adolescence was found to lead to a lower risk of cardiovascular death two generations later [6]. Enriched environments may also induce a favourable transgenerational response, with better performance or a better protective or compensatory response in cases of misprogramming [8]. Finally, interactions between the father and mother and between the young and their mother are relevant [9-11]. The responses observed in descendants may therefore be diverse and may differ from the effects of the initial impact on the parent, with anything from the vicious circle most frequently reported to an adaptation opening up new possibilities.

 

Sexual dimorphism and non-genetic heritability

Studies of gene expression and epigenetic marks and modifications have revealed the existence of different mechanisms of environmental adaptation in males and females, in both humans and model animals [11-15]. The effects of, and responses to, programming may affect offspring of both sexes, or may affect one of the sexes more than the other [7, 16, 17]. Furthermore, depending on the nature of the environment, and the developmental window and duration of exposure, the sex of the transmitting parent may also condition the response of the offspring to the environment. Following exposure (or an absence of exposure) to toxic substances, alcohol, or under- or overnutrition, during a particular developmental window or after weaning, certain phenotypic characteristics may be inherited solely from the father, solely from the mother, or from both parents equally [11, 18, 19]. The Overkalix cohort provides a good illustration of these differences. The risk of cardiovascular disease and diabetes in a man or a woman is dependent on the abundance or lack of food to which the grandparents, but only the paternal grandparents, were exposed before puberty, [13]. The information is transmitted by the paternal grandfather to his grandsons, but not to his granddaughters. Similar results have been reported for rodents, for undernutrition or the consumption of areca nuts [11, 20]. The transmission of behavioural characteristics from the father to his female descendants, but not to his male descendants, has also been observed in genetically identical mice displaying phenotypic heterogeneity in terms of behaviour [21].

Exposure to certain environments may affect the germline of the father or mother (or both), all their somatic tissues, and their reproductive systems, including the genital tract and environment. This results in a complex dialogue between these systems that may lead to concerted transfer to subsequent generations [10, 13, 14, 22](Figure 2)

epigenetics-3

Figure 2. Schematic pedigree diagram showing the main routes for the biological transmission of the effects of exposure to the subsequent generations
Left, female line; right, male line. The exposure can potentially affect the germline, the reproductive system and the somatic tissues. The   traditional pedigree lines (blue) show chromosomal transmission, with the possibility of exposure-induced epigenetic marks that escape erasure and affect the development of the offspring. The germline can potentially transmit exposure-induced non-coding RNAs (ncRNAs) that influence offspring development. Exposure-induced metabolic changes can set up a ‘metabolic cascade’, such that changes in the reproductive tract influence early embryonic programming of the offspring or change metabolic signals across the placenta. An additional maternal route of transmission is the influence of the mother’s microbiome on that of her child. From [13].

 

 

The germline and gametes may display genetic (XX or XY), ontogenetic, morphological and functional differences between the sexes. The non-genetic differences result from epigenetic asymmetry, which may continue after fertilization [23, 24]. At conception, the gametes deliver the genetic heritage, DNA, which forms the genome of the embryo. They also transmit the different epigenomes and RNA molecules from both the father and mother, and mitochondria and a number of proteins from the mother only. Thus, in addition to the genetic heritage of the embryo, the parents also provide epigenetic, protein-based and metabolic information relating to exposure to environmental factors, experience, physiopathological state, age, social class, parental education, and birth rank and weight [11, 25].

Maternal transmission has traditionally been the most widely studied, but mostly as concerns inter- or multigenerational responses in terms of embryonic or foetal growth and development during gestation or lactation (Figure 1). Many maternal physiological conditions, not necessarily involving the germline, have been studied: metabolic conditions, nutrition, exposure to toxic substances or stress or free choice to mate with attractive males [4, 26-31]. Epigenetic transmission via the maternal line has been demonstrated in rodents [32-34], but it is generally difficult to distinguish what has been transmitted by the gamete from what has been transmitted through the materno-fetal unit during gestation. By contrast, studies of paternal transmission, although less common, have raised questions about the mechanisms by which spermatozoa transmit information: possibilities include via epigenomic marks, non-coding RNA or the seminal fluid [10, 11, 13, 25] (Figure 3).

epigenetics-4

Figure 3. Illustration of the non-genomic pathways through which paternal effects on offspring development may occur. The experiences of males (drugs, nutrition, toxins, age, stress), particularly during early development, may lead to epigenetic alterations in the male germline (red circle), which are then transmitted to the offspring with consequences for phenotypic variation. Alternatively, or probably in combination with these direct paternal effects, the experiences of a male before mating may lead to changes in mate quality or preference, which may be assessed by the female at the time of mating. This assessment may lead to differences in maternal prenatal and/or postnatal investment in the growth and development of the offspring generated from this mating, with consequences for phenotypic variation in the offspring. Maternal investment may also vary with paternally mediated variations in offspring phenotype, during both the prenatal and postnatal periods. Differences in maternal investment as a function of paternal experiences or offspring traits may either enhance transmission of the memory of paternal exposure or compensate for deficits in functioning due to that exposure. From [11]. 

 

 

The various phases of reprogramming and the twists of mark erasure

Two principal phases of reprogramming, the erasure of parental marks, have been studied: the first occurs in the zygote, just after fertilization, and the others occur in the germline, when the primordial germ cells migrate towards the genital ridges before sexual differentiation (Figure 4a and 4c) [35].

epigenetics-5

Figure 4. Epigenetic changes during in vivo reprogramming
(a) DNA methylation dynamics during developmental reprogramming. After fertilisation, the paternal genome (blue line) is rapidly demethylated by active mechanisms, whereas the maternal genome (red line) is passively demethylated. Differentially methylated regions (DMRs) associated with imprinted genes are protected from this erasure (dashed green line). De novo methylation occurs after implantation (black line), but primordial germ cells (PGCs) are not specified until the epiblast stage (shading at the top of the figure). This methylation must be reset in PGCs. The figure shows the methylation dynamics, from E6.5, of the cells forming the germline only. Most sequences are demethylated by E9.5 in PGCs. Some sequences are subject to late demethylation and are not reprogrammed until after PGC migration. These sequences include, but are not limited to, the imprinted DMRs. IAPs Intracisternal A particles are resistant to demethylation during both the post-fertilisation and PGC waves of reprogramming. Variably erased CG islands (VECs) can resist erasure during PGC reprogramming, but their methylation status during post-fertilisation reprogramming is unclear. Following sex determination, the germ cells undergo de novo methylation, but the dynamics are sex-specific. Methylation is completed in the prospermatogonia before birth, whereas methylation in the oocytes is established during postnatal growth. In adulthood, the gametes are appropriately methylated to form a new zygote and to restart the cycle of methylation dynamics. We show below the developmental windows investigated in three key studies, with the specific time points analyzed indicated. blast., blastocyst. d5, day 5 oocytes. GV, germinal vesicle oocytes. MII, metaphase II oocytes. From [35].
(b). Epigenetic changes during in vivo reprogramming. Schematic diagram of the global DNA and histone modifications leading to transcriptional activation of the embryonic genome between the late zygote (paternal genome only) and the two-cell stage. Gamete genomes undergo different epigenetic programs after fertilisation, with the paternal genome mostly subject to epigenetic remodeling at the zygote stage and the maternal genome gradually losing repressive modifications during subsequent cleavages. (b) Global epigenetic changes during germline development from PGC specification (E6.5) to mitotic/meiotic arrest at E13.5. Two major reprogramming phases can be distinguished during PGC migration toward the genital ridges (E7.5–E10.5) and upon their arrival in the gonads (E10.5–E12.5). From [57]
(c). Global epigenetic changes during germline development from PGC specification (E6.5) to mitotic/meiotic arrest at E13.5. Two major reprogramming phases can be distinguished during PGC migration toward the genital ridges (E7.5–E10.5) and upon their arrival in the gonads (E10.5–E12.5). From [57]

 

 

The reprogramming of the parental genomes in the zygote was long considered to be almost complete. However, the histone marks and methylation patterns of certain DNA sequences may not be erased [36]. Two other phases may also be considered as reprogramming processes: the final compaction of the chromatin of the spermatozoa, linked to the replacement of a large proportion of the histones by protamines (Figure 4b) and the massive changes, particularly during the reorganization of the brain and its maturation during puberty. This last phase of reprogramming has not yet been studied in detail [37].

One of these phases, involving the erasure of specific epigenetic marks from the gametes, leads to the acquisition of a totipotent epigenome, allowing the cells of the embryo to differentiate into any type of cell (Figure 4b). Some sequences, such as those of genes subject to parental imprinting, escape this process. Following another phase of reprogramming linked to the germline, the remethylation of DNA occurring after sex determination facilitates the acquisition of a very specific expression programme, including imprinted genes, for gamete differentiation (Figure 4c). Given the epigenetic asymmetry of the gametes of the father and the mother, sensitive unerased marks may differ between chromosomes of paternal and maternal origin in the zygote. The mechanisms involved have yet to be determined, but these observations suggest that the possibilities for transmission may differ as a function of the transmitting parent [38] (Figure 5).

epigenetics-6

Figure 5. Life cycle of mammalian gametogenesis and embryogenesis
Primordial germ cells (PGCs) arise from proximal epiblast cells. They undergo extensive erasure of DNA methylation and chromatin changes during migration to and entry into the gonad. Directed by the somatic gonadal environment, germ cells are destined for a male or female fate. Male germ cells, initially called gonocytes, have arrested cell cycles and they begin to establish male-specific DNA methylation patterns. During the subsequent meiotic prophase, the X and Y chromosomes undergo meiotic sex chromosome inactivation (MSCI) characterised by major chromatin remodelling events. Following meiotic divisions, haploid spermatids undergo extensive nuclear and morphological changes, including an almost genome-wide replacement of histones with protamines. However, nucleosomes are retained on regulatory sequences, providing a potential means of epigenetic inheritance. Female germ cells enter meiotic prophase in the embryo and complete their meiotic divisions upon hormonal induction in the adult ovary and fertilisation by sperm. During the growing phase, oocytes establish DNA methylation at genes and imprinting control regions, undergo chromatin remodelling and acquire competence for the direction of embryogenesis. Upon fertilisation, the parental genomes form two pronuclei that are epigenetically different, reflecting the history of parental germline-specific chromatin remodelling events. The paternal and maternal genomes undergo active and passive erasure of DNA methylation. The asymmetry of the chromatin states of paternal and maternal chromosomes may potentially regulate the activation and repression of de novo gene expression in pre-implantation embryos, thereby directing embryogenesis. A latent epigenetic state, characterised by the presence of H3K4me3 and H3K27me3 bivalent marks in the promoters of genes involved in development, not expressed at these stages, is a fundamental property of the nucleus of mammalian germline cells, enabling differentiated gametes to initiate a totipotency programme immediately after fertilisation [58]. From [38].

 

 

The incomplete erasure of some parental epigenetic marks — DNA methylation and histone marks — and the polycomb and trithorax systems make possible the programming and transgenerational transmission of environmental impacts [36, 39]. These regions are thus ideal candidates for the transfer of environmental exposure information. Depending on the chromosome concerned, and particularly if the X and Y chromosomes display such differences, could these regions account for the differences in the effects on female and male offspring? The principal problem here is that the epigenetic mechanisms involved are dynamic and change rapidly with environmental variation. They are also based on multiple strata of partially redundant pathways, which may be synergic, inhibitory or activating, depending on the context [40, 41]. Thus, if the impact of the exposure of the future parents to environmental factors several years before conception affects precisely this type of sequence [4, 13, 14, 42], then these sequences may be responsible for the transgenerational responses observed in the descendants.

However, probably for technical reasons potentially linked to the composition of histone variants, the various studies performed did not identify the same types of sequence. The nucleosomes identified were located principally at genes critical for early or late development [43], and at regulatory sequences, but some were also found at repeated sequences containing few genes [44]. These sequences are potential candidates for epigenetic heredity.

 

Mysterious intermediaries passing on the message from generation to generation

At fertilisation, in both humans and mice, there are many more methylated sites in the spermatozoa than in the oocyte [45]. There is extensive, but site-specific demethylation in the male and female pronuclei after fertilization. This process involves both active and passive mechanisms, depending on the parental origin of the chromosome [45]. It is widely accepted that only imprinted genes escape this process of demethylation. However, one recent study showed that other genes are also resistant [46]. In this mouse model of undernutrition in the grandmother (F0), the spermatozoa of the father (F1) display a disturbance of the methylome in differentially methylated regions (DMRs), with effects on the metabolism of his descendants (F2) [46]. Interestingly, 43% of the DMRs hypomethylated in the F1 were also hypomethylated in the F2 generation and therefore had the potential to affect the development of this subsequent generation. Many of the genes affected are expressed in the germline, but some are also expressed in somatic tissues. However, although this differential methylation was lost from the F2 generation by the end of gestation, major differences persisted in the expression of genes involved in metabolism located close to these DMRs. It therefore seems unlikely that these changes in expression are directly controlled by DNA methylation [46]. A similar process has been reported for the repercussions in the second generation of the effects of diet-induced maternal obesity [47]. These examples show that epigenetic profiles deregulated early in development are capable of passing the torch to other entities, thereby inducing other, as yet unidentified changes that might affect chromatin architecture, networks of transcription factors, or the differentiation or structure of tissues. In the model of resistance to cocaine addiction, the same modification (histone acetylation) to the same gene (Bdnf) was observed in the spermatozoa of the father and in the prefrontal cortex of his resistant male progeny [48]. As histone acetylation is a mark associated with expression, this observation cannot be seen as proof that this is the mechanism responsible for information transfer. The two examples cited above only appear to be contradictory; they do not in any way exclude the possible involvement of an epigenetic process. The pertinent epigenetic marks involved have probably either not been studied or have not been studied at the appropriate stage. Given the dialogue known to occur between marks, we would expect more than one type of mark to be involved, together with other, non-epigenetic processes. Are these associations the cause or a consequence of the dynamics of these marks? The key question to be addressed here remains that of the true causal link between epigenetic marks and the observed phenotypes.

 

Non-coding RNAs

During fertilisation, the spermatozoid not only provides the paternal haploid genome, it also releases 24,000 non-coding RNAs (ncRNAs: siRNA, piRNA and miRNA…) into the oocyte. Sperm RNA has been shown to transmit acquired characters in rodents. In particular, the use of sperm from maltreated animals has been shown to reproduce metabolic or behavioural changes in the descendants similar to those observed in the father [5, 26, 32, 34, 49-51].

One recent report suggested that RNA isolated from sperm might provide the progeny with information about the history of precocious trauma (through maternal stress) in the life of the father, with the effects and responses persisting until the third generation [5]. However, once again, the absence of presumed causal epigenetic alteration suggests that the initial mark may be transposed to other marks or epigenetic complexes in a relay. The epigenetic modifications present in the sperm cells following exposure to maternal stress may thus be converted into other marks, which may or may not be epigenetic in nature, for subsequent transmission [30, 52].

The involvement of ncRNA in transgenerational effects and responses was recently demonstrated in an invertebrate species lacking DNA methylation, C. elegans [27]. Exposure to viral particles led to the appearance of ncRNAs derived from the virus, which inhibited the expression of the viral genome, by RNA interference mechanisms, over several generations, thereby conferring a transmissible “immunity” [53]. A lack of food during the larval stage also leads to the appearance of microRNAs (miRNAs) targeting transcripts for proteins involved in nutrition and leading to an increase in the longevity of the third generation. These miRNAs cope with all eventualities, as some also target genes that are normally switched off but may be induced in response to stress [54].

 

Perspectives

The influences of environmental factors on epigenetic processes have revolutionised our view of the transgenerational transmission of information, but several key questions remain unanswered: What is the true nature of the impact of environmental factors? What is the nature of the targets of these factors (marks and/or conformation)? What is the nature of the targets to which the information is transferred? Are the mechanisms involved direct or indirect? How does the stored information persist over generations? What are the windows of sensitivity or insensitivity to these factors? How do differences linked to the sex of the parents impose sexual dimorphism on the progeny and, even, on subsequent generations [4, 13, 26 , 29, 55]?

There is still no federative model for the role of epigenetics in inter- and transgenerational effects/responses[29]. Ideally, before concluding that a transgenerational effect is epigenetic in nature, given the two-directional relationships between genetics and epigenetics, sequencing should be carried out to check for de novo mutations; similarly, in vitro fertilisation and embryo transfer or cross-adoption should be carried out, to control for other possibilities, such as maternal investment induced by the father. Such experiments are possible in animal models, but much more difficult in humans. The genes and sequences escaping reprogramming and the mechanisms involved are beginning to be identified and are good candidates for involvement in transgenerational effects. Studies of the effects of the environment would make it possible to determine whether these sequences carry a memory of these effects or whether other sequences can obtain the same capacity to resist mark erasure. By contrast, the processes, epigenetic or otherwise, by which the information is propagated are unknown, as are those underlying the differences in transmission from the father and the mother. Above all, very few studies have focused on the effects of the environment on these processes, to determine how the memory of events can be transmitted and to reveal the nature of the successive intermediary supports. Most reprogramming studies have been carried out in mice [35, 56]. The conservation of certain mechanisms between species opens up interesting possibilities.

The author is grateful to Polina Panchenko, Sara Fneich, Luciano Pirola, Sabrina Chriett, Valérie Amarger, Bertrand Kaeffer, Patricia Parnet, Jérome Torrisani, Francisco Bolaños Jimenez, Hélène Jammes and, Anne Gabory, for their careful reading of the manuscript.

  1. Junien C, al e: Le nouveau paradigme de l’Origine développementale de la santé et des maladies (DOHaD), Epigénétique, Environnement : preuves et chaînons manquants. Medecine Sciences 2015.
  2. Barker DJP, Osmond C: Infant mortality, childhood nutrition and ischaemic heart disease in England and Wales. The Lancet 1986, 327:1077-1081.
  3. Yan W: Potential roles of noncoding RNAs in environmental epigenetic transgenerational inheritance. Mol Cell Endocrinol 2014, 398:24-30.
  4. Grossniklaus U, Kelly WG, Ferguson-Smith AC, Pembrey M, Lindquist S: Transgenerational epigenetic inheritance: how important is it? Nat Rev Genet 2013, 14:228-235.
  5. Gapp K, Jawaid A, Sarkies P, Bohacek J, Pelczar P, Prados J, Farinelli L, Miska E, Mansuy IM: Implication of sperm RNAs in transgenerational inheritance of the effects of early trauma in mice. Nat Neurosci 2014, 17:667-669.
  6. Kaati G, Bygren LO, Edvinsson S: Cardiovascular and diabetes mortality determined by nutrition during parents’ and grandparents’ slow growth period. Eur J Hum Genet 2002, 10:682-688.
  7. Attig L, Vige A, Gabory A, Karimi M, Beauger A, Gross MS, Athias A, Gallou-Kabani C, Gambert P, Ekstrom TJ, et al: Dietary alleviation of maternal obesity and diabetes: increased resistance to diet-induced obesity transcriptional and epigenetic signatures. PLoS One 2013, 8:e66816.
  8. Arai JA, Feig LA: Long-lasting and transgenerational effects of an environmental enrichment on memory formation. Brain Res Bull 2011, 85:30-35.
  9. Junien C: L’empreinte parentale : de la guerre des sexes à la solidarité entre générations. Médecine/Sciences 2000, 3:336-344.
  10. Bromfield JJ, Schjenken JE, Chin PY, Care AS, Jasper MJ, Robertson SA: Maternal tract factors contribute to paternal seminal fluid impact on metabolic phenotype in offspring. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2014, 111:2200-2205.
  11. Curley JP, Mashoodh R, Champagne FA: Epigenetics and the origins of paternal effects. Horm Behav 2011, 59:306-314.
  12. Junien C, Gabory A, Attig L: [Sexual dimorphism in the XXI(st) century]. Med Sci (Paris) 2012, 28:185-192.
  13. Pembrey M, Saffery R, Bygren LO: Human transgenerational responses to early-life experience: potential impact on development, health and biomedical research. J Med Genet 2014, 51:563-572.
  14. Lane M, Robker RL, Robertson SA: Parenting from before conception. Science 2014, 345:756-760.
  15. Dunn GA, Morgan CP, Bale TL: Sex-specificity in transgenerational epigenetic programming. Horm Behav 2010, 59:290-295.
  16. Drake AJ, Walker BR: The intergenerational effects of fetal programming: non-genomic mechanisms for the inheritance of low birth weight and cardiovascular risk. J Endocrinol 2004, 180:1-16.
  17. Anderson LM, Riffle L, Wilson R, Travlos GS, Lubomirski MS, Alvord WG: Preconceptional fasting of fathers alters serum glucose in offspring of mice. Nutrition 2006, 22:327-331.
  18. Dunn GA, Bale TL: Maternal high-fat diet effects on third-generation female body size via the paternal lineage. Endocrinology 2011, 152:2228-2236.
  19. Anway MD, Skinner MK: Epigenetic programming of the germ line: effects of endocrine disruptors on the development of transgenerational disease. Reprod Biomed Online 2008, 16:23-25.
  20. Martinez D, Pentinat T, Ribo S, Daviaud C, Bloks VW, Cebria J, Villalmanzo N, Kalko SG, Ramon-Krauel M, Diaz R, et al: In utero undernutrition in male mice programs liver lipid metabolism in the second-generation offspring involving altered Lxra DNA methylation. Cell Metab 2014. Jun 3;19(6):941-51
  21. Alter MD, Gilani AI, Champagne FA, Curley JP, Turner JB, Hen R: Paternal transmission of complex phenotypes in inbred mice. Biol Psychiatry 2009, 66:1061-1066.
  22. Alminana C, Caballero I, Heath PR, Maleki-Dizaji S, Parrilla I, Cuello C, Gil MA, Vazquez JL, Vazquez JM, Roca J, et al: The battle of the sexes starts in the oviduct: modulation of oviductal transcriptome by X and Y-bearing spermatozoa. BMC Genomics 2014, 15:293.
  23. Hackett JA, Surani MA: Beyond DNA: programming and inheritance of parental methylomes. Cell 2013, 153:737-739.
  24. Duffie R, Bourc’his D: Parental epigenetic asymmetry in mammals. Curr Top Dev Biol 2013, 104:293-328.
  25. Rando OJ: Daddy issues: paternal effects on phenotype. Cell 2012, 151:702-708.
  26. Daxinger L, Whitelaw E: Understanding transgenerational epigenetic inheritance via the gametes in mammals. Nat Rev Genet 2012, 13:153-162.
  27. Lim JP, Brunet A: Bridging the transgenerational gap with epigenetic memory. Trends Genet 2013, 29:176-186.
  28. Aiken CE, Ozanne SE: Transgenerational developmental programming. Hum Reprod Update 2014, 20:63-75.
  29. Heard E, Martienssen RA: Transgenerational epigenetic inheritance: myths and mechanisms. Cell 2014, 157:95-109.
  30. Drake AJ, Seckl JR: Transmission of programming effects across generations. Pediatr Endocrinol Rev 2011, 9:566-578.
  31. Gowaty PA, Anderson WW, Bluhm CK, Drickamer LC, Kim YK, Moore AJ: The hypothesis of reproductive compensation and its assumptions about mate preferences and offspring viability. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2007, 104:15023-15027.
  32. Rassoulzadegan M, Grandjean V, Gounon P, Vincent S, Gillot I, Cuzin F: RNA-mediated non-Mendelian inheritance of an epigenetic change in the mouse. Nature 2006, 441:469-474.
  33. Weiss IC, Franklin TB, Vizi S, Mansuy IM: Inheritable effect of unpredictable maternal separation on behavioral responses in mice. Front Behav Neurosci 2011, 5:3.
  34. Wagner KD, Wagner N, Ghanbarian H, Grandjean V, Gounon P, Cuzin F, Rassoulzadegan M: RNA induction and inheritance of epigenetic cardiac hypertrophy in the mouse. Dev Cell 2008, 14:962-969.
  35. Cowley M, Oakey RJ: Resetting for the next generation. Mol Cell 2012, 48:819-821.
  36. Holland ML, Rakyan VK: Transgenerational inheritance of non-genetically determined phenotypes. Biochem Soc Trans 2013, 41:769-776.
  37. Morrison KE, Rodgers AB, Morgan CP, Bale TL: Epigenetic mechanisms in pubertal brain maturation. Neuroscience 2014, 264:17-24.
  38. Gill ME, Erkek S, Peters AH: Parental epigenetic control of embryogenesis: a balance between inheritance and reprogramming? Curr Opin Cell Biol 2012, 24:387-396.
  39. Hajkova P, Erhardt S, Lane N, Haaf T, El-Maarri O, Reik W, Walter J, Surani MA: Epigenetic reprogramming in mouse primordial germ cells. Mech Dev 2002, 117:15-23.
  40. Riising EM, Comet I, Leblanc B, Wu X, Johansen JV, Helin K: Gene silencing triggers polycomb repressive complex 2 recruitment to CpG islands genome wide. Mol Cell 2014, 55:347-360.
  41. Festenstein R, Chan JP: Context is everything: activators can also repress. Nat Struct Mol Biol 2012, 19:973-975.
  42. Brydges NM, Jin R, Seckl J, Holmes MC, Drake AJ, Hall J: Juvenile stress enhances anxiety and alters corticosteroid receptor expression in adulthood. Brain Behav 2014, 4:4-13.
  43. Hammoud SS, Nix DA, Zhang H, Purwar J, Carrell DT, Cairns BR: Distinctive chromatin in human sperm packages genes for embryo development. Nature 2009, 460:473-478.
  44. Saitou M, Kurimoto K: Paternal nucleosomes: are they retained in developmental promoters or gene deserts? Dev Cell 2014, 30:6-8.
  45. Smith ZD, Chan MM, Humm KC, Karnik R, Mekhoubad S, Regev A, Eggan K, Meissner A: DNA methylation dynamics of the human preimplantation embryo. Nature 2014, 511:611-615.
  46. Radford EJ, Ito M, Shi H, Corish JA, Yamazawa K, Isganaitis E, Seisenberger S, Hore TA, Reik W, Erkek S, et al: In utero effects. In utero undernourishment perturbs the adult sperm methylome and intergenerational metabolism. Science 2014, 345:1255903.
  47. King V, Dakin RS, Liu L, Hadoke PW, Walker BR, Seckl JR, Norman JE, Drake AJ: Maternal obesity has little effect on the immediate offspring but impacts on the next generation. Endocrinology 2013, 154:2514-2524.
  48. Vassoler FM, White SL, Schmidt HD, Sadri-Vakili G, Pierce RC: Epigenetic inheritance of a cocaine-resistance phenotype. Nat Neurosci 2013, 16:42-47.
  49. Saab BJ, Mansuy IM: Neuroepigenetics of memory formation and impairment: The role of microRNAs. Neuropharmacology 2014, 80C:61-69.
  50. Liu WM, Pang RT, Chiu PC, Wong BP, Lao K, Lee KF, Yeung WS: Sperm-borne microRNA-34c is required for the first cleavage division in mouse. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2012, 109:490-494.
  51. Abramowitz LK, Bartolomei MS: Genomic imprinting: recognition and marking of imprinted loci. Curr Opin Genet Dev 2012, 22:72-78.
  52. Sharma A: Bioinformatic analysis revealing association of exosomal mRNAs and proteins in epigenetic inheritance. J Theor Biol 2014, 357:143-149.
  53. Rechavi O, Minevich G, Hobert O: Transgenerational inheritance of an acquired small RNA-based antiviral response in C. elegans. Cell 2011, 147:1248-1256.
  54. Rechavi O: Guest list or black list: heritable small RNAs as immunogenic memories. Trends Cell Biol 2014, 24:212-220.
  55. Gabory A, Roseboom TJ, Moore T, Moore LG, Junien C: Placental contribution to the origins of sexual dimorphism in health and diseases: sex chromosomes and epigenetics Biol Sex Differ 2013, Mar 21;4(1):5. [Epub ahead of print].
  56. Reik W, Kelsey G: Epigenetics: Cellular memory erased in human embryos. Nature 2014, 511:540-541.
  57. Cantone I, Fisher AG: Epigenetic programming and reprogramming during development. Nat Struct Mol Biol 2013, 20:282-289.
  58. Lesch BJ, Dokshin GA, Young RA, McCarrey JR, Page DC: A set of genes critical to development is epigenetically poised in mouse germ cells from fetal stages through completion of meiosis. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2013, 110:16061-16066.

 

0 Comments

Leave a reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

*

Send this to a friend